THE BIGGEST
PUBLIC HEALTH EXPERIMENT EVER: The 1954 Field Trial of the Salk Poliomyelitis
Vaccine
Paul
Meier University of Chicago
The largest
and most expensive medical experiment in history was carried out in 1954. Well over a million young children
participated, and the immediate direct costs were over 5 million dollars. The experiment was carried out to assess the
effectiveness, if any, of the Salk vaccine as a protection against paralysis or
death from poliomyelitis. The study was
elaborate in many respects, most prominently in the use of placebo controls
(children who were inoculated with simple salt solution) assigned at random
(that is, by a carefully applied chance process that gave each volunteer an
equal probability of getting vaccine or salt solution) and subjected to a
double-blind evaluation (that is, an arrangement under which neither the
children nor the physicians who evaluated their subsequent state of health knew
who had been given vaccine and who got the salt solution).
Why was such elaboration necessary? Did it really result in more or better
knowledge than could have been obtained from much simpler studies? These are the questions on which this
discussion is focused.
BACKGROUND
Polio was never a common disease, but it
certainly was one of the most frightening and, in many ways, one of the most
inexplicable in its behavior. It struck
hardest at young children, and, although it was responsible for only about 6%
of the deaths in the age group 5 to 9 in the early fifties, it left many
helpless cripples, including some who could survive only in a respirator. It appeared in epidemic waves, leading to
summer seasons in which some communities felt compelled to close swimming pools
and restrict public gatherings as cases increased markedly from week to week;
other communities, escaping an epidemic one year, waited in trepidation for the
year in which their turn would come.
Rightly or not, this combination of selective attack upon the most
helpless age group and the inexplicable vagaries of its epidemic behavior, led
to far greater concern about polio as a cause of death than other causes, such
as auto accidents, which are more frequent and, in some ways, more amenable to
community control. The determination
to mount a major research effort to eradicate polio arose in no small part from
the involvement of President Franklin D. Roosevelt, who was struck down by
polio when a successful young politician.
His determination to overcome his paralytic handicap and the commitment
to the fight against polio made by Basil O'Connor, his former law partner,
enabled a great deal of attention, effort, and money to be expended on the care
and rehabilitation of polio victims and-in the end, more importantly-on
research into the causes and prevention of the disease. During the course of this research, it was
discovered that polio is caused by a virus and that three main virus types are
involved. Although clinical
manifestations of polio are rare, it was discovered that the virus itself was
not rare, but common, and that most adult individuals had experienced a polio
infection sometime in their lives without ever being aware of it. This finding helped to explain the
otherwise peculiar circumstance that polio epidemics seemed to hit hardest
those who were better off hygienically (i.e., those who had the best nutrition,
most favorable housing conditions, and were otherwise apparently most favorably
situated). Indeed, the disease seemed
to be virtually unknown in those countries with the poorest hygiene. The explanation is that because there was
plenty of polio virus in the less-favored populations, almost every infant was
exposed to the disease early in life while he was still protected by the immunity
passed on from his mother. As result,
everyone had had polio, but under protected circumstances, and thereby,
everyone had developed his own immunity.
As with many other virus diseases, an
individual who has been infected by polio and recovered is usually immune to
another attack (at least by a virus strain of the same type). The reason for this is that the body, in
fighting the infection, develops antibodies, which are a part of the gamma
globulin fraction of the blood, to the antigen, which is the protein part of
the polio virus. These antibodies
remain in the bloodstream for years, and even when their level declines so far
as to be scarcely measurable, there are usually enough of them to prevent a
serious attack from the same virus.
Smallpox and influenza illustrate two different approaches to the
preparation of an effective vaccine.
For smallpox, which has long been controlled by a vaccine, we use for
the vaccine a closely related virus, cowpox, which is ordinarily incapable of
causing serious disease in man, but which gives rise to antibodies that also
protect against smallpox. (In a very
few individuals this vaccine is capable of causing a severe, and occasionally
fatal, reaction. The risk is small enough,
however, so that we do not hesitate to expose all our school children to it in
order to protect them from smallpox.) In the case of influenza, however,
instead of a closely related live virus, the vaccine is a solution of the
influenza virus itself, prepared with a virus that has been killed by treatment
for a time with formaldehyde. Provided
that the treatment is not too prolonged, the dead virus still has enough
antigenic activity to produce the required antibodies so that, although it can
no longer infect, it is, in this case, sufficiently like the live virus to be a
satisfactory vaccine. In the case of
polio, both of these methods were explored.
A live-virus vaccine would have the advantage of reproducing in the
vaccinated individual and, hopefully, giving rise to a strong reaction which
would produce a high level of long-lasting antibodies. With such a vaccine '
however, there might be a risk that a vaccine virus so similar to the virulent
polio virus could mutate into a virulent form and itself be the cause of
paralytic or fatal disease. A
killed-virus vaccine should be safe because it presumably could not infect, but
it might fail to give rise to an adequate antibody response. These and other problems stood in the way of
the rapid development of a successful vaccine.
Some unfortunate prior experience also contributed to the cautious
approach of researchers. In the
thirties, attempts had been made to develop vaccines against polio; two of
these were actually in use for a time.
Evidence that at least one of these vaccines, in fact, had been
responsible for cases of paralytic polio soon caused both to be promptly
withdrawn from use. This experience
was very much in the minds of polio researchers, and they had no wish to risk a
repetition. Research to develop both
live and killed vaccines was stimulated in the late forties by the development
of a tissue culture technique for growing polio virus. Those working with live preparations
developed harmless strains from virulent ones by growing them for many
generations in suitable tissue culture media.
There was, of course, considerable worry lest these strains, when used
as a vaccine in man, might revert to virulence and cause paralysis or
death. (By 1972 it seems clear that the
strains developed are indeed safe-a live-virus preparation taken orally is the
vaccine presently in widespread use throughout the world.) Those working with
killed preparations, notably Jonas Salk, had the problem of treating the virus
(with formaldehyde) sufficiently to eliminate its infectiousness, but not so
long as to destroy its antigenic effect.
This was more difficult than, at first, had appeared to be the case, and
some early lots of the vaccine proved to contain live virus capable of causing
paralysis and death. There are
statistical issues in the safety story (Meier 1957), but our concern here is
with the evaluation of effectiveness.
EVALUATION OF EFFECTIVENESS
In the early fifties the Advisory Committee
convened by the National Foundation for Infantile Paralysis (NFIP) decided that
the killed-virus vaccine developed by Jonas Salk at the University of
Pittsburgh had been shown to be both safe and capable of inducing high levels
of the antibody in children on whom it had been tested. This made the vaccine a promising candidate
for general use, but it remained to prove that the vaccine actually would pre-
vent polio in exposed individuals. It
would be unjustified to release such a vaccine for general use without
convincing proof of its effectiveness, so it was determined that a large-scale "field
trial" should be undertaken. That
the trial had to be carried out on a very large scale is clear. For suppose we wanted the trial to be
convincing if indeed the vaccine were 50% effective (for various reasons, 100%,
effectiveness could not be expected).
Assume that, during the trial, the rate of occurrence of polio would be
about 50 per 100,000 (which was about the average incidence in the United
States during the fifties). With 40,000
in the control group and 40,000 in the vaccinated group, we would find about 20
control cases and about 10 vaccinated cases, and a difference of this magnitude
could fairly easily be attributed to random variation. It would suggest that the vaccine might be
effective, but it would not be persuasive.
With 100,000 in each group, the expected numbers of polio cases would be
50 and 25, 'and such a result would be persuasive. In practice, a much larger study was clearly required, because it
was important to get definitive results as soon as possible, and if there were relatively
few cases of polio in the test area, the expected number of cases might be well
under 40. It seemed likely, also, for
reasons we shall discuss later, that paralytic polio, rather than all polio,
would be a better criterion of disease, and only about half the diagnosed cases
are classified "paralytic." Thus the relatively low incidence of the
disease, and its great variability from place to place and time to time,
required that the trial involve a huge number of subjects-as it turned out,
over a million.
THE VITAL STATISTICS APPROACH
Many modern therapies and vaccines, including
some of the most effective ones, such as smallpox vaccine, were introduced
because preliminary studies suggested their value. Large-scale use subsequently provided clear evidence of
efficacy. A natural and simple approach
to the evaluation of the Salk vaccine would have been to distribute it as
widely as possible, through the schools, to see whether the rate of reported
polio was appreciably less than usual during the subsequent season. Alternatively, distribution might be limited
to one or a few areas because limitations of supply would preclude effective
coverage of the entire country. There
is even a fairly good chance that were one to try out an effective vaccine
against the common cold or against measles, convincing evidence might be
obtained in this way. In the case of
polio-and, indeed, in most cases-so simple an approach would almost surely fall
to produce clear cut evidence. First,
and foremost, we must consider how much polio incidence varies from season to
season, even without any attempts to modify it. From Figure 1, which shows the annual reported incidence from
1930 through 1955, we see that had a trial been conducted in this way in 1931,
the drop in incidence from 1931 to 1932 would have been strongly suggestive of
a highly effective vaccine because the incidence dropped to less than a third
of its previous level. Similar
misinterpretations would have been made in 1935, 1937, and other years-most recently
in 1952. (On the general problem of
drawing inferences from such time series data see the essay by Campbell.) One
might suppose that such mistakes could be avoided by using the vaccine in one
area, say, New York State, and comparing the rate of incidence there with that
of an unvaccinated area, say, Illinois. Unfortunately, an epidemic of polio
might well occur in Chicago-as it did in 1956-during a season in which New York
had a very low incidence. Another
problem, more subtle, but equally burdensome, relates to the vagaries of
diagnosis and reporting. There is no
difficulty, of course, in diagnosing the classic respirator case of polio, but
the overwhelming majority of cases are less clearcut. Fever and weakness are common symptoms of many illnesses,
including polio, and the distinction between weakness and slight transitory
paralysis will be made differently by different observers. Thus the decision to diagnose a case as
nonparalytic polio instead of some other disease may well be influenced by the
physician's general knowledge or feeling about how widespread polio is in his
community at the time. These
difficulties can be mitigated to some extent by setting down very precise
criteria for diagnosis, but it is virtually impossible to obviate them
completely when, as would be the case after the widespread introduction of a
new vaccine, there is a marked shift in what the physician expects to
find. This is most especially true
when the initial diagnosis must be made by family physicians who cannot easily
be indoctrinated in the use of a special set of criteria, as is the case with
polio. Later evaluation by specialists
cannot, of course, bring into the picture those cases originally diagnosed as
something other than polio.
THE OBSERVED CONTROL APPROACH
The difficulties of the vital statistics
approach were recognized by all concerned, and the initial study plan, although
not judged entirely satisfactory, got around many of the problems by
introducing a control group similar in characteristics to the vaccinated group. More specifically, the idea was to offer
vaccination to all children in the second grade of participating schools and to
follow the polio experience not only in these children, but in the first- and
third-grade children as well. Thus the
vaccinated second-graders would constitute the treated group, and the
first- and third-graders would constitute the control group. This plan follows what we call the observed
control approach. It is clear
that this plan avoids many of the difficulties that we listed above. The three grades all would be drawn from the
same geographic location so that an epidemic affecting the second grade in a
given school would certainly affect the first and third grades as well. Of course, all subjects would be observed
concurrently in time. The grades,
naturally, would be different ages, and polio incidence does vary with
age. Not much variation from grade to
grade was expected, however, so it seemed reasonable to assume that the average
of first and third grades would provide a good control for the second
grade. Despite the relative
attractiveness of this plan and its acceptance by the NFIP advisory committee,
serious objections were raised by certain health departments that were expected
to participate. In their judgment, the
results of such a study were likely to be insufficiently convincing for two
important reasons. One is the
uncertainty in the diagnostic process mentioned earlier and its liability to
influence by the physician's expectations, and the other is the selective
effect of using volunteers. Under the
proposed study design, physicians in the study areas would have been aware of
the fact that only second-graders were offered vaccine, and in making a
diagnosis for any such child, they would naturally and properly have inquired
whether he had or had not been vaccinated.
Any tendency to decide a difficult diagnosis in favor of nonpolio when
the child was known to have been vaccinated would have resulted in a spurious
piece of evidence favoring the vaccine.
Whether or not such an effect was really operating would have been
almost impossible to judge with assurance, and the results, if favorable, would
have been forever clouded by uncertainty.
A less conjectural difficulty lies in the difference between those
families who volunteer their children for participation in such a trial and
those who do not. Not at all
surprisingly, it was later found that those who do volunteer tend to be better
educated and, generally, more well-to-do than are those who do not
participate. There was also evidence
that those who agree to participate tend to be absent from school with a
noticeably higher frequency than others.
The direction of effect of such selection on the incidence of diagnosed
polio is by no means clear before the fact, and this important difference
between the treated group and the control group also would have clouded the
interpretation of the results.
RANDOMIZATION AND THE PLACEBO CONTROL
APPROACH
The position of critics of the NFIP plan was
that the issue of vaccine effective- ness was far too important to be studied
in a manner which would leave uncertainties in the minds of reasonable
observers. No doubt, if the vaccine
should appear to have fairly high effectiveness, most public health officials
and the general public would accept it, despite the reservations. If, however, the observed control scheme
were used, a number of qualified public health scientists would have remained
unconvinced, -and the value of the vaccine would be uncertain. Therefore, the critics proposed that the
study be run as a scientific experiment with the use of appropriate randomizing
procedures to assign subjects to treatment or to control and with a maximum
effort to eliminate observer bias. This
plan follows what we call the placebo control approach.
The chief objection to this plan was that
parents of school children could not reasonably be expected to permit their
children to participate in an experiment in which they might be getting only an
ineffective salt solution instead of a probably helpful vaccine. It was argued further that the injection of
placebo might not be ethically sound, since a placebo injection carries a small
risk, especially if the child unknowingly is already infected with polio. The proponents of the placebo control
approach maintained that, if properly approached, parents would consent
to their children's participation in such an experiment, and they judged that
because the injections would not be given during the polio season, the risk
associated with the placebo injection itself was vanishingly small. Certain health departments took a firm
stand: they would participate in the trial only if it were such a well-designed
experiment. The consequence was that in
approximately half the areas, the randomized placebo control method was used,
and in the remaining areas, the alternating-grade observed control method was
used.
A major effort was put forth to eliminate any
possibility of the placebo control results being contaminated by subtle
observer biases. The only firm way to
accomplish this was to insure that neither the subject, nor his parents, nor
the diagnostic personnel could know which children had gotten the vaccine until
all diagnostic decisions had been made.
The method for achieving this result was to prepare placebo material
that looked just like the vaccine, but was without any antigenic activity, so
that the controls might be inoculated and otherwise treated in just the same
fashion as were the vaccinated. Each vial
of injection fluid was identified only by a code number so that no one involved
in the vaccination or the diagnostic evaluation process could know which
children had gotten the vaccine.
Because no one knew, no one could be influenced to diagnose differently
for vaccinated cases and for controls.
An experiment in which both the subject getting the treatment and the
diagnosticians who will evaluate the outcome are kept in ignorance of the
treatment given each individual is called a double-blind experiment. Experience in clinical research has shown
the double-blind experiment to be the only satisfactory way to avoid
potentially serious observer bias when the final evaluation is in part a matter
of judgment. For most of us, it is
something of a shock to be told that competent and dedicated physicians must be
kept in ignorance lest their judgments be colored by knowledge of treatment
status. We should keep in mind that it
is not deliberate distortion of findings by the physician which concern the
medical experimenter. It is rather the
extreme difficulty in many cases of making an uncertain decision which,
experience has shown, leads the best of investigators to be subtly influenced
by information of this kind. For
example, in the study of drugs used to relieve postoperative pain, it has been found
that it is quite impossible to get an unbiased judgment of the quality of pain
relief, even from highly qualified investigators, unless the judge is kept in
ignorance of which patients were given which drugs.
The second major feature of the experimental
method was the assignment of subjects to treatments by a careful randomization
procedure. As we ob- served earlier,
the chance of coming down with a diagnosed case of polio varies with a great
many factors including age, socioeconomic status, and the like. If we were to make a deliberate effort to
match up the treatment and control groups as closely as possible, we should
have to take care to balance these and many other factors, and, even so, we
might miss some important ones.
Therefore, perhaps surprisingly, we leave the balancing to a carefully
applied equivalent of coin tossing: we arrange that each individual has an
equal chance of getting vaccine or placebo, but we eliminate our own judgment
entirely from the individual decision and leave the matter to chance. The gain from doing this is twofold. First, a chance mechanism usually will do a
good job of evening out all the variables-those we didn't recognize in advance,
as well as those we did recognize.
Second, if we use a chance mechanism in assigning treatments, we may be
confident about the use of the theory of chance, that is to say, probability
theory, to judge the results. We can
then calculate the probability that so large a difference as that observed
could reasonably be due solely to the way in which subjects were assigned to
treatments, or whether, on the contrary, it is really an effect due to a true
difference in treatments. To be sure,
there are situations in which a skilled experimenter can balance the groups
more effectively than a random-selection procedure typically would. When some factors may have a large effect on
the outcome of an experiment, it may be desirable, or even necessary, to use a
more complex experimental design that takes account of these factors. However, if we in- tend to use probability
theory to guide us in our judgment about the results, we can be confident about
the accuracy of our conclusions only if we have used randomization at some
appropriate level in the experimental design.
The final determinations of diagnosed polio proceeded along the
following lines. First, all cases of
poliolike illness reported by local physicians were subjected to special
examination, and a report of history, symptoms, and laboratory findings was
made. A special diagnostic group then
evaluated each case and classified it as nonpolio, doubtful polio, or definite
polio. The last group was subdivided
into nonparalytic, paralytic, and fatal polio.
Only after this process was complete was the code broken and
identification made for each case as to whether vaccine or placebo had been
administered.
RESULTS OF
THE TRIAL
The main results are shown in Table 1, which
shows the size of the study populations, the number of cases classified as
polio, and the disease rates, that is, the number of cases per 100,000
population. For example, the second
line shows that in the placebo control area there were 428 reported cases of
which 358 were confirmed as polio, and among these, 270 were classified as
paralytic (including 4 that were fatal).
The third and fourth rows show corresponding entries for those who were
vaccinated and those who received placebo, respectively. Beside each of these numbers is the
corresponding rate. Using the simplest
measure-all reported cases-the rate in the vaccinated group is seen to be half
that in the control group (compare the boxed rates in Table 1) for the placebo
control areas. This difference is
greater than could reasonably be ascribed to chance, according to the
appropriate probability calculation.
The apparent effectiveness of the vaccine is more marked as we move from
reported cases to paralytic cases to fatal cases, but the numbers are small and
it would be unwise to make too much of the apparent, very high effectiveness in
protecting against fatal cases. The
main point is that the vaccine was a success; it demonstrated sufficient
effectiveness in pre- venting serious polio to warrant its introduction as a
standard public health procedure. Not
surprisingly, the observed control area provided results that were, in general,
consistent with those found in the placebo control area. The volunteer effect discussed earlier,
however, is clearly evident (note that the rates for those not inoculated
differ from the rates for controls in both areas). Were the observed control information alone available,
considerable doubt would have remained about the proper interpretation of the
results. Although there had been wide
differences of opinion about the necessity or desirability of the placebo control
design before, there was great satisfaction with the method after the
event. The difference between the two
groups, although substantial and definite, was not so large as to preclude
doubts had there been no placebo controls.
Indeed, there were many surprises in the more detailed data. It was known, for example, that some lots of
vaccine had greater antigenic power than did others, and it might be supposed
that they should have shown a greater protective effect. This was not the case; lots judged inferior
in antigenic potency did just as well as those judged superior. Another surprise was the rather high
frequency with which apparently typical cases of paralytic polio were not
confirmed by laboratory test.
Nonetheless, there were no surprises of a character to cast serious
doubt on the main conclusion. The
favorable reaction of those most expert in re- search on polio was expressed
soon after the results were reported. By
carrying out this kind of study before introducing the vaccine, it was noted,
we now have facts about Salk vaccine that we still lack about typhoid vaccine,
after 50 years of use, and about tuberculosis vaccine, after 30 years of use.
TABLE 1: Summary of Study Cases by
Diagnostic Class and Vaccination Status (Rates per 100,000)
|
_______________Poliomyelitis Cases________________________ |
||||||||||||
|
|
All
Reported ___Cases___ |
___Total___ |
Paralytic |
Nonparalytic |
Fatal
polio |
Not
Polio |
||||||
STUDY
GROUP |
STUDY
POPULATION |
No. |
Rate |
No. |
Rate |
No. |
Rate |
No. |
Rate |
No. |
Rate |
No. |
Rate |
All
areas: Total |
1,829,916 |
1,1013 |
55 |
863 |
47 |
685 |
37 |
178 |
10 |
15 |
1 |
150 |
8 |
Placebo
control areas: Total |
749,236 |
428 |
57 |
358 |
48 |
270 |
36 |
88 |
12 |
4 |
1 |
70 |
9 |
Vaccinated |
200,745 |
82 |
41 |
57 |
28 |
33 |
16 |
24 |
12 |
0 |
0 |
25 |
12 |
Placebo |
201,229 |
162 |
81 |
142 |
71 |
115 |
57 |
27 |
13 |
4 |
2 |
20 |
10 |
Not
inoculated* |
338,778 |
182 |
54 |
157 |
46 |
121 |
36 |
36 |
11 |
0 |
0 |
25 |
7 |
Incomplete
vaccinations |
8,484 |
2 |
24 |
2 |
24 |
1 |
12 |
1 |
12 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
Observed
control areas: Total |
1,080,680 |
585 |
54 |
505 |
47 |
415 |
38 |
90 |
8 |
11 |
1 |
80 |
7 |
Vaccinated |
221,998 |
76 |
34 |
56 |
25 |
38 |
17 |
18 |
8 |
0 |
0 |
20 |
9 |
Controls† |
725,173 |
439 |
61 |
391 |
54 |
330 |
46 |
61 |
8 |
11 |
2 |
48 |
6 |
Grade
2 not inoculated |
123,605 |
66 |
54 |
54 |
44 |
43 |
35 |
11 |
9 |
0 |
0 |
12 |
10 |
Incomplete
vaccinations |
9,904 |
4 |
40 |
4 |
40 |
4 |
40 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
0 |
* Includes
8,577 children who received one or two injections of placebo.
† First- and
third-grade total population.
Source:
Adapted from Francis (1955), Tables 2 and 3
EPILOGUE
It would be
pleasant to report an unblemished record of success for the Salk vaccine,
following so expert and successful an appraisal of its effectiveness, but it is
more realistic to recognize that such success is but one step in the continuing
development of public health science.
The Salk vaccine, although a notable triumph in the battle against
disease, was relatively crude and, in many ways, not a wholly satisfactory
product that was soon replaced with better ones. The report of the field trial was followed by widespread release
of the vaccine for general use, and it was discovered very quickly that a few
of these lots actually had caused serious cases of polio. Distribution of the vaccine was then halted
while the process was reevaluated.
Distribution was reinitiated a few months later, but the momentum of
acceptance had been broken and the prompt disappearance of polio that
researchers hoped for did not come about.
Meanwhile, research on a more highly purified killed- virus vaccine and
on several live-virus vaccines progressed, and within a few years the Salk
vaccine was displaced by live-virus vaccines.
The long-range historical test of the Salk vaccine, in consequence, has
never been carried out. We do not know
with certainty whether or not that vaccine could have accomplished the
relatively complete elimination of polio that has now been achieved. Nonetheless, this does not diminish the
importance of its role in providing the first heartening success in the attack
on this disease, a role to which careful and statistically informed
experimental design contributed greatly.
PROBLEMS
1.
Using Figure
I as an example, explain why a control group is needed in experiments where the
effectiveness of a drug or vaccine is to be determined.
2. Explain the need for control groups by criticizing the
following statement: "A study on the benefits of vitamin C showed that 90%
of the people suffering from a cold who take vitamin C get over their cold
within a week."
3. Explain the difference between the observed control approach and the placebo
control approach. Which one would you
prefer, and why?
4. Why is it important to have a "double-blind" experiment?
5. If "double-blind" experiments provide the only satisfactory way to
avoid observer bias, why aren't they used all the time?
6. If only volunteers are used in an experiment, instead of a random sample of
individuals, will the results of the experiment be of any value? What can you say about the results?
7. Why did the polio epidemics seem to hit hardest those who were better off
hygienically?
8. Why was a large-scale field trial needed to get convincing evidence of the
Salk vaccine effectiveness?
9. Refer to Figure 1. In which year did the highest polio incidence occur? the lowest? the largest increase?
the smallest increase? Give the
approximate values of these incidences and increases.
10. Refer to Figure 1. Comment on the use of the number of cases. Can you suggest a different indicator of
the spread of poliomyelitis in the U.S. during 1930-56. When are the two indicators equivalent? (Hint: refer to Table 1.)
REFERENCES
1. K. Alexander Brownlee. 1955. "Statistics of the 1954 Polio Vaccine
Trials." journal of the American Statistical Association 50(272),
1005-1013.
2.
Thomas Francis, Jr., et al. 1955.
"An Evaluation of the 1954 Poliomyelitis Vaccine Trials--Sumnmary Report."
American Journal of Public Health 45(5), 1-63.
3.
Paul Meier. 1957. "Safety
Testing of Poliomyelitis Vaccine." Science 125(3,257),
1067-1071.
4.
D. D. Rutstein. 1957. "How
Good is Polio Vaccine? " Atlantic
Monthly 199,48-51.